Day
5: The Ultimate Riddle
Again,
Tropical Storm Andrea has kept the rain right above us here in Charlottesville,
VA. Today was originally scheduled to be the Monticello Field School’s
fieldtrip to the archaeological excavations in the Coastal Plane. On the
original agenda was visiting the excavations at James Fort, on Jamestown
Island, the Jamestown Rediscovery Project, the Association for the Preservation
of Virginia Antiquities, and the Colonial Williamsburg Foundation. The
fieldtrip also included a visit to the excavations at the Bray School Site (an
eighteenth century African-American school), but due to the rain, Dr. Neiman
was able to move our trip to Monday but unfortunately without the Bray School
visit on the agenda.
The
lecture this morning brought a lot of fascinating information and theories to
the table; it dove deeper into the multiple facets of archaeology, which
included functionalism. The lecture began on the topic of household archaeology
when there are no houses. Households are defined as a unit of economic and
social cooperation, and are formed through cooperation, co-residence, and
enterprise. In the case of slave houses however, cooperation was not
necessarily founded under any one roof, co-residents formed communities of
practice, and thus created and a community through mutual engagement in a joint
enterprise defined and sustained through practice, these communities developed
out of ways which people participated in actions whose meanings are negotiated
with others (cooperation). Functionalism, a term taken from cultural sociology,
into archaeology, in hopes of creating a new field where how people worked and
lived in the household setting is determined from the artifacts found. Each individual
person in a society may have different takes on what is happening around them. Households
are natural social and economic communities, cooperation should naturally
occur, but no framework of cooperation for when people choose to cooperate or
when they refuse not to. This brings into play the Prisoner’s Dilemma attributed
to John Nash (see chart below).
It breaks down into an individual chooses
between cooperation or defect, their choice is effected by the other individual’s
choice, and the payoffs are symmetrical. The lecture went into the deep
mathematical material needed to work out the probability, interactions, and
payoffs, which I will not cover in this blog. The key concept of the Prisoner’s
Dilemma is the overall success of the defection will spread throughout the
group (or community) and the defectors will takeover- yet, this result does not
have the community’s interest at heart. A population of defection has a payoff
of one, whereas a population of cooperation has a payoff of 3, ultimately
causing the community to get the worst payoff.
Similar
to today’s community formations (i.e. diaspora communities), several mechanisms
for correlation in the Prisoner’s Dilemma can be found, and easily understood.
Kinship, or “kin selection,” meaning if my best friend chooses to cooperate, I
will choose to cooperate as well. Direct Reciprocity, meaning the conditional
cooperation based on personal experience, such as a group of friend choosing
the same strategy in order to purposefully correlate with one another. Indirect
Reciprocity, is the conditional cooperation based on social reputation, a
simple example is gossip, knowing other’s social reputations before deciding to
defect or cooperate with them. In the case of slavery, most of them had been
ripped from the original kinship networks of their home countries. The slave
process also randomizes people in the slave community together, into new,
forced communities. So how can enslaved people re-establish cooperative bonds
within their community?
Finally,
here we start to come full circle to the previous material of the fieldschool
(and the blog), to the sub-floor pit (“Hidey holes”) occurrences in the
Chesapeake region and the reasons behind them. Slaves were placed with
unrelated individuals in housing, meaning random interactions, and cooperation
was formed through social reputation of others behaviors, these pits represent
one way to make other people’s reputations known (holding someone accountable
if they stole from your sub-floor pit), and that in turn makes cooperation with
others easier. In the case of multiple pits, the slaves chose not to share a
single pit, again because it was a random grouping, making the reputations of
others (cooperation or defection) unknown. An enslaved person would likely
trust the community to stop someone from stealing from their pit, or holding
them accountable, then risking sharing a pit with a random individual whose
reputation is unknown.
From
here the lecture transition into house size and cost. At Monticello, Jefferson
began as a tobacco farmer and housed his slaves in multiple large log cabins
near the tobacco fields, and later when he switched to wheat farming, he began
to place his slaves in multiple smaller log cabins. Partially this was for
farming convenience of the smaller wheat crops, but it was also for economic
purposes. Three smaller log cabins took 262lbs. of Chestnut wood to build, with
dimensions of total perimeters of 156ft. and a total area of 96ft, the single
large log cabin took 675lbs. of Chestnut, 504ft. total perimeter and 504ft of
total area. Rather than build three small log cabins, Jefferson knew it was
logically cheaper (by 111lbs. of Chestnut) to build one big structure for his
slaves and group them together at random as tobacco working gangs. Later, when
Jefferson switched to wheat farming, he allows the slaves to live in the
smaller, more expensive to build houses, and in these smaller houses the
sub-floor pits simply disappear. Is this because the slaves were allowed to choose
who they had to live with, of course choosing people they trusted or were
related to, or is this one of the incentives Jefferson offered if the slaves
behaved without an overseer (his wheat fields were too far placed and Jefferson
did not want to have to hire more overseers =$$)? If the slaves behaved, could
they have been allowed to choose who they lived with?
Now
we drift back into the subject, household archaeology without the houses, and
creating a research design focusing on an archaeological spatial structure. For
our example, we used “Site 8" which dates from 1770-1800 here at Monticello. Through correspondence analysis can be combined into a single stratified sample, meanin one concise data representation from 266 five-foot excavated quadrats. In order to effectively analyze
the data collected from each quadrat, this correspondence analysis is necessary.
Correspondence analysis takes six dimensions and shrinks it down into more manageable
data for us as archaeologists to use and make inferences, but also for future
archaeologists to observe.
The
data that was seen from the 266 excavated quadrats presented fascinating images
to hypothesize about. Within Site 8, a north and a south community are present.
These two communities move further and further apart on site, yet share similar
ceramic popularity. This could represent two families simply expanding or
just shared trade networks of two separate communities. So far in the southern
group no pits have been found yet or they don’t have them, whereas the north
group has pits and the size of them increases over time. The ceramic remains (sherds)
found across the site, in both the north and the south, provide some very compelling
evidence as well. The ceramic density is larger in the North area of Site 8 and
small in the south, the ceramic artifact size is small in the south (small
sherds) and large in the north. Ceramics are bigger in the south but found in
larger quantity in the northern area of Site 8. This could suggest a difference
between yard maintenance attitudes between the north and south communities. Perhaps
the north cleaned up their space and properly disposed of their trash, and the
south did not. Ceramics is not the only instance where this correlation occurs
(remember, consistency is key for archaeological science). Whole nails are
found in the south area (didn’t clean up) and broken nails are found in the
north.
To
close the day, we stated our tentative conclusions about Site 8. We believe the
southern community to be an early occupation date, and the north to be a later
settlement. Later on in time, the north group moves further north, building
bigger, separate sub-floor pits. The south group has no pits. This may be a
sampling error, a sign of poverty, or a sign that they were the dominating
community between the two. There is no site maintenance in the southern
community, they were collectively messy, and acted as though they may pick up and
leave tomorrow (vs. what may be settled families in the north community). Are
these simply transient individuals or a high resident turnover rate? Only time
will tell. The fun part (and somewhat frustrating) about archaeology, is that
all of these observations and inferences about the Site 8 north and south
communities could be absolutely wrong, and tomorrow may hold the find to answer
the riddle. SO WE DIG!
No comments:
Post a Comment